UNIVERSAL NORMS - VARY WITH CULTURE
NORMS – AN OVERVIEW
|
Social norms are the behaviors and cues within a
society or group. This sociological term has been defined as "the rules
that a group uses for appropriate and inappropriate values, beliefs, attitudes
and behaviors. These rules may be explicit or implicit. Failure to follow the
rules can result in severe punishments, including exclusion from the
group." They have also been described as the "customary rules of
behavior that coordinate our interactions with others."
The social norms indicate the established and
approved ways of doing things, of dress, of speech and of appearance. These
vary and evolve not only through time but also vary from one age group to
another and between social classes and social groups. What is deemed to be
acceptable dress, speech or behavior in one social group may not be accepted in
another. The remarkably wide diversity of attitudes and behaviors from one
culture to another indicates the extent to which we are the products of
cultural norms. Norms restrain and
control us, but they also lubricate the social machinery. Social behavior
occurs with greater ease when everyone knows what is both expected and accepted.
Despite their distinct differences, cultures share some norms in common. One apparently universal norm concerns how
people of unequal status relate to one another.
The more formal way we communicate with strangers is the same way we
communicate with superiors. Moreover,
increased intimacy is usually initiated by person with higher status. Other
norms vary across cultures.
WHAT ARE THE UNIVERSAL NORMS?
Although norms vary by culture and genders, we
humans do hold some norms is common.
Best known is the taboo against incest; Parents are not to have sexual
relations with their children, nor siblings with one another. Although the taboo apparently is violated
more often than psychologists once believed, the norm is still universal. Every society disapproves of incest.
Roger Brown noticed another universal norm.
Everywhere, people talk to higher-status people in the respectful way they
often talk to strangers. And they talk to lower-status people in the more
familiar, first name way they speak to friends.
Patients call their physician “Dr. So and So”; the physician often
replies using their first name. Students and professors typically address o9ne
another in a similar non-mutual way. Most languages have two forms of the
English pronoun “you”: a respectful form and a familiar form (for example, Sie
and due in German, vous and tu in French, usted an tu in Spanish, aap and tum
in Pakistan). People typically sue the
familiar form with intimates and those they perceive as inferior (not only with
close friends and family members but also in speaking to children and
dogs). A German child received a boost
when strangers begin addressing the child as “Sie” instead of “du”. Nouns, too, can express assumed social
inequalities. Among faculty studies by
Rebecca Rubin, young female professors were far more likely that young male
professors to have students call them by their first name. Women tennis players will empathize”
Sportscasters 53 percent of the time refer to them using only their first name:
with men players this happens only 8 percent of the time.
This first aspect of Brown’s universal norm – that
forms of address communicate not only social distance but also social
status—correlates with a second aspect:
Advances in intimacy are usually suggested by the higher status person.
In Europe, where most twosomes begin a relationship with the polite, formal
“you” and may eventually progress to the more intimate “you,” someone obviously has to initiate the
increased intimacy. Who do you suppose
does so? On some congenial occasion, the
elder, or richer, or more distinguished of the two may say, “Why don’t we say
du to one another.
This norm extends beyond language to every type of
ad once in intimacy. It is more
acceptable to borrow a pen from or put a hand on the shoulder of one’s
intimates and subordinates than to behave in such a casual way with strangers
or superiors. Similarly, the president
of my college invites faculty to his home before they invite him to theirs. In general, then, the higher status person is
the pacesetter in the progression toward intimacy.
NORMS VARY WITH CULTURE
Some norms cross cultures, but many do not. To someone from a relatively formal northern
European culture, a person whose roots are in a expressive Mediterranean culture
may seem “warm, charming, inefficient, and time wasting.” To the Mediterranean person, the northern
European may seem “efficient, cold, and over concerned with time”. Latin American business executives who arrive
late for a dinner engagement may be mystified by how obsessed their American
counterparts are with punctuality.
Culture also vary in their norms for personal space,
a sort of portable bubble or buffer zone that we like to maintain between
ourselves and others. As the situation
changes, the bubble varies in size.
With strangers we maintain a fairly large personal space, keeping a
distance of 4 feet or more between us.
On un-crowded buses, or in restrooms or libraries, we protect our space
and respect others’ space. We let
friends come closer, often within 2 or 3 feet.
Individuals differ:
Some people prefer more personal space than others. Adults maintain more distance those
children. Men keep more distance form
one another than do women. For reasons
unknown, cultures near the equator prefer less space and more touching and
hugging. Thus the British and
Scandinavians prefer more distance than the French and Arabs; Americans prefer
more space than Latin Americans.
To see the effect of encroaching on another’s
personal space, play space invader.
Stand or sit but a foot or so from a friend and strike up a
conversation. Does the person fidget,
look away, back off, show other signs of discomfort? These are some signs of arousal noted by
researchers. One controversial
experiment revealed that men require more time to begin urination and less time
to complete the act when another man is standing at an adjacent urinal.
VARY WITH GENDER
There are many obvious dimensions of human
diversity—height, weight, hair, color, to name just few. But for people’s self-concepts and social
relationships, the two dimensions that matter most, and that people first
attune to, are race and, especially, sex.
As it has perhaps, the sexes are nearly identical in
many physical traits, such as age of sitting, teething, and walking. They also
are alike in many psychological traits, such as overall vocabulary,
intelligence, and happiness. But the occasional differences are what capture
attention and make news. Women are twice s vulnerable to anxiety disorders an
depression, though only one-third as vulnerable to alcoholism and suicide. Women have a slightly better sense of smell.
Thy more easily become re-aroused immediately after orgasm. And they are much less likely to suffer
hyperactivity or speech disorders as children and to display antisocial
personalities as adults.
During the 1970’s, many scholars worried that
studies of gender differences might reinforce stereotypes and that gender
differences might be construed as women’s deficits. Focusing attention on gender differences will
provide “battle weapons against women” they warned. And it’s true that explanations for
differences usually focus on the group that’s seen as different. In discussing the “gender gap” in American presidential voting, commentators
more often wonder why women so often
vote Democratic than why men so often vote Republican. People more often wonder
what causes homosexuality than what causes heterosexuality. People ask why
Asian-Americans so often excel in math and science, not whey other groups less
often excel. In each case, people define
the standard by one group and wonder why the other is “different” From
“different” it is but a short way to “deviant” or “sub-standard.”
Since the 1980’s scholars have felt freer to explore
gender diversity. Some argued that
gender difference research has “furthered the cause of gender equality” by
reducing overblown stereotypes and that we can accept and value gender
diversity. The two sexes, like two sides
of a coin, can differ yet be equal. Let’s consider, then, two dimensions of gender
diversity in social norms.
GLOBAL MULTICULTURAL CIVIL SOCIETY
Within
a global multicultural civil society, tolerance is constituted by both the
recognition of cultural differences and group identities on the one hand and
respect for universal moral norms on the other. A humanist morality and
politics has to reconcile adequately universal norms and group values. This is
possible if one understands moral diversity as an answer to the problem of
social cooperation. As a result, the recognition of plurality is preserved
without giving up an essential trait of all humanist morality which is the
objective validity of universal norms. There is a general idea inherited from
the enlightenment which is not confined to the European and especially French
history in the eighteenth century, but which is common to all humanist periods
of human thinking and social development in many cultures and in different
periods of time: This is the idea of equality, and the related idea that there
are certain rights, intrinsic values and duties which are not bound to special
social relationships, memberships of certain groups, ethnicities or nations,
and which together build a framework of moral constraints within which all
morally acceptable behaviour is embedded. It is the fundamental assumption of
genuine political conservatism that those universal norms do not exist.
Certainly there are many modern conservatives who do not hold this assumption
anymore, which was essential for Edmund Burke and other critics of the French
enlightenment and the French revolution. However, those have given up an
essential trait of conservative politics which justifies an understanding of
this kind of "conservatism" as a form of liberalism combined with the
empirical judgment that certain genuinely liberal values can be uphold only if
the dynamics of change in society is limited.
Today
a large part of political thought within the Left is also openly
anti-universalist and partly even anti-humanist. Even moderate proponents of
multiculturalism easily give up constitutive elements of normative humanism and
seem to revitalize exactly those normative elements which have been
constitutive for political conservatism. This new collectivist and
anti-universalist tendency is - like orthodox Marxism has been in the past - a
result of (and the response to) certain deficiencies of mainstream liberal
thought.
This
common normative framework, constitutive for interaction, is obviously
compatible with different individual goals, values and subjective moralities. Compare
this with our judgment not concerning moral questions but descriptive ones. To
participate in successful communication presupposes a wide range of common
assumptions. Different opinions about a certain subject matter can be
identified only if there is enough consensus concerning other subject matters
such that the dissenting parties can understand each other. This is true for
empirical and moral disputes as well. There is a common element between moral
disagreement and other disagreements regarding questions of rationality or
scientific facts: The disagreement is a genuine one. A non genuine disagreement
might arise if I prefer staying at home and my partner prefers going out. Then
we realize that we have conflicting preferences in case we both want to spend this day together. But if we
begin to discuss how to solve this conflict, e. g. by referring to a principle of fairness ("last time
you decided, this time I should have the right to decide"), then we might
reach an agreement by a more fundamental consensus regarding the rule which we
want to apply. Otherwise we might go on disagreeing what to do, but then this
disagreement has turned into a genuine one: One thinks it is fair that p and
the other thinks it is fair that q, and p and q are incompatible.
Comments
Post a Comment