TECHNIQUES OF PERSUASION AND NEW DIRECTION
TECHNIQUES OF PERSUASION
Three techniques those are common
in persuasive communication-humor, sex, and repetition. The research on the
first two suggests that they be used carefully, because they can be
misunderstood or can distract from the message, and there is little evidence
that they actually bring about attitude change. Repetition has its pros and
cons and should also be used carefully. It increases the chances of penetrating
through audience indifference or resistance, and it can lead to greater
learning—of a message, of a relationship between a product name and positive
associations, or of the connection between a credible source and particular
message. But it can also lead to increased counter arguing and increased
thinking about other, irrelevant topics. Newer theories of persuasion grant a
more active role to the receiver; information-integration theory is one of the
more popular of these new theories. Audiences and communicators need to
understand their applications—and their potential misuse.
ü Appeal to Humor
ü Appeals to Sex
ü Effects of Repetition
Appeals to Humor: The use of
humor is a popular technique in communication. Many public speakers obviously
believe in the importance of beginning their talks with a humorous story.
Studies have suggested that 15 to 20 percent of television commercials contain
some element of humor (Kelly & Solomon, 1975; Duncan & Nelson, 1985).
When attitude change or persuasion
is the dependent variable of interest, most studies have not found a
significant effect due to humor (Gruner, 1965, 1967, 1970, 1972; Brooker,
1981).
Other studies of the effectiveness
of humor indicate that it has more of an effect on lower-order communication
effects (responses lower in the response hierarchy) than on higher-order
communication effects (Gelb & Pickett, 1983; Duncan & Nelson, 1985).
That is, humor is more effective in attracting attention, generating liking for
the communicator, and so forth, than it is in producing attitude change or
changes in behavior.
Not all studies agree, however,
that humor is even effective in generating liking for the communicator. One
study showed that a woman speaker was liked less when she used humor than when
she did not (Taylor, 1974). The author suggests that the speaker was perceived
as "trying too hard to curry favor." Similarly, another study showed
that college teachers who use humor are perceived with "suspicion and
hostility" because they are acting contrary to student expectations that a
teacher's behavior will be controlling and evaluative (Darling & Civikly,
1984). The research on the effectiveness of humor that has been conducted so
far should be interpreted in light of its limitations, however. One limitation
is that the settings of the studies have often been classrooms or laboratories,
which might not be representative of the settings where humor is expected.
Another limitation is that the research has tended to be nontheoretical, with
little discussion of why humor might or might not be effective in achieving
various effects. Markiewicz (1974) has suggested that learning theory and distraction
theory are two promising theories for understanding the relationship of humor
to persuasion. A learning theory approach might suggest that humor would
provide reinforcement and thus lead to greater attitude change. A distraction
theory approach might make the prediction that humor would be distracting. This
distraction, in turn, might lead to greater attitude change by preventing counter
arguing (Festinger & Maccoby, 1964). Or distraction might lead to less
attitude change by interfering with attentiveness to the message.
Appeals to Sex: The use of sexy models and other sexual appeals is a common
technique in advertising. One study has indicated that more than one-fourth of
magazine ads contain "obviously alluring" female models (Sexton &
Haberman, 1974). Furthermore, these kinds of ads are on the increase. The same
study showed the ads with "obviously alluring" models increased from
10 percent in 1951 to 27 percent in 1971. Many advertisers apparently believe
that "sex sells." But does it?
At least one study suggests that a
sexy model can affect the perception or image of a product, even if there is
very little logical connection between the model and the product. Smith and
Engel (1968) prepared a print ad for an automobile in two versions. In one
version, a female model clad in
black lace panties and a simple sleeveless sweater stood in front of the car.
She held a spear—on the assumption that the spear might be regarded as a
phallic symbol and might lead the model to be seen as more aggressively
seductive. In the other version, there was no model. When the car was pictured
with the woman, subjects rated it as more appealing, more youthful, more
lively, and better designed. Even objective characteristics were affected. When
the car appeared with the woman, it was rated as higher in horsepower, less
safe, more expensive by $340, and able to move an average 7.3 miles per hour
faster. In general, male and female subjects responded the same way to the ads.
In contrast to the Smith and Engel
study, however, a number of studies investigating the effects of sexy models on
brand recall have shown either no effect or less recall with the sexy model
(Chestnut, LaChance, & Lubitz, 1977; Alexander & Judd, 1978). It
appears that the sexy models distract the viewers' attention away from the
portion of the ad presenting the product or company name.
One study suggests that for certain
products, an attractive female might not be as effective in stimulating sales
as an attractive male (Caballero & Solomon, 1984). This study changed the
displays for a brand of beer and a brand of tissue that appeared at the end of
an aisle in a Tom Thumb supermarket. They found that overall, the male models
tended to stimulate more beer sales among both male and female customers than
either the female stimulus or the control (no model) treatment.
Another study indicates that
response to sex in advertising is not a simple variable (Mor-rison &
Sherman, 1972). The study had a number of subjects look at ads from magazines
and express their reaction to them on rating scales. They then used cluster
analysis to look for different patterns of response. For the males, they
identified three dimensions of response as important: (1) the "Tom
Jones" dimension, (2) the intellectualizing dimension, and (3) the
fetishism dimension. For females, they identified four dimensions: (1) sensualism,
(2) love/sexism, (3) romanticism, and (4) fantasism. Not only do different ads
emphasize some of these dimensions and not others, but different people respond
to some dimensions favorably while ignoring others completely. The authors also
found that, contrary to their expectation, women were more quick to pick up on
suggestiveness in copy than men.
There are also some clear-cut age
differences in responses to sex appeals in advertising, with younger people
approving of them more than older people (Wise, King, & Merenski, 1974). Even though sex in advertising is common, it appears that
there are some risks in using it. Appeals to sex might be disapproved of by
some audience members, might be misperceived or missed by others, and might
distract still others from the real purpose of the ad. Few, if any, studies
exist that show a positive effect of sex in advertising on brand recall or
product sales. While the Smith and Engel study shows a sexy model having the
effect of increasing the favorable evaluation of an automobile in an ad, it did
not test for brand recall after seeing the ad. It is possible that the subjects
did not recall the name of the kind of automobile any better with the sexy
model than without, and this would defeat the purpose of the ad. Jib Fowles, the author of an article titled
"Advertising's Fifteen Basic Appeals," draws this conclusion about
appeals to the need for sex: "As a rule, though, advertisers have found
sex to be a tricky appeal, to be used sparingly. Less controversial and equally
fetching are the appeals to our need for affectionate human contact"
(1982, p. 278).
Effects of Repetition: Many mass
communication messages—particularly advertisements, whether commercial or
political—are repeated extensively. There are a number of reasons why this
might be a good idea. Not all audience members will be watching at the same
time, or, in the print media, not all readers will see a single printing of an
advertisement. Another advantage of repetition is that it might remind the
audience of a source for a message from a high-credibility source, and thus
prevent the drop-off in attitude change from a high-credibility source found
over time by Hovland and Weiss. Repeating a message might help the learning of
attitudes and emotional meanings for words discussed by Staats and Staats,
since a repeated association of the two stimuli is part of the process of
conditioning. Repetition might help the audience remember the message itself.
Zielske (1959) showed that advertising is quickly forgotten if not
continuously exposed.
Krugman (1972) has presented the
intriguing argument that three exposures might be all that are needed for a
television advertisement to have its desired effect. But he adds the important
qualification that it might take 23 exposures to get the three that produce the
particular responses that are needed. Krugman suggests that the first exposure
to an ad is dominated by a cognitive "What is it?" response. The
second exposure is dominated by an evaluative "What of it?" response.
And the third exposure is a reminder, but also the beginning of disengagement.
Krugman points out a fundamental difficulty, however, in that people can screen
out television ads by stopping at the "What is it?" response without
further involvement. Then, on perhaps the 23rd exposure, they might, or might
not, move on to the "What of it?" response. Thus, Krugman's analysis
is stating that three exposures to an advertisement might be enough under ideal
circumstances, but that it might take a number of repetitions to achieve those
three.
Too much repetition can also have
some undesirable effects, however. In one study, three groups of subjects were
presented with one, three, or five repetitions of a persuasive message
(Cacioppo & Petty, 1979). The researchers found that the message repetition
led at first to increasing agreement with the advocated position, but that
after a certain point it led to decreasing agreement with the advocated
position. They found repetition led to decreasing, then increasing, counter
arguing against the message by the recipient of the message. And they found
that any amount of repetition led to increasing topic-irrelevant thinking. This
kind of curvilinear relationship between repetition and communication effects
was also found in a study of political advertising. Becker and Doolittle (1975)
found that both liking for a candidate and seeking of information about a
candidate were highest with a moderate amount of repetition but declined with
high repetition.
Another study found that humor
ratings declined steadily with repetition of ads (Gelb & Zinkham, 1985). A
change in the creative execution of the ad was found to boost the humor ratings
back up, though.
NEW DIRECTIONS IN PERSUASION THEORY:
Probably the major change in the
field of persuasion theory in recent years has been a move away from a
mechanistic stimulus-response view of persuasion to a view that recognizes the
active role of the receiver. As Raymond S. Ross puts it, "In our view,
persuasive messages attempt to influence how receivers choose or decide which information to process.
This implies the utility of strategy and theory and the criticality of audience
analysis". (1985) Taking essentially the same view, Mary John Smith has
stated, "A process of persuasion has occurred when people internalize the
meanings they assign to messages in an atmosphere of perceived choice"
(1982). This approach is sometimes called a transactional approach to
persuasion. It emphasizes the choice of the receiver.
One point at which we see the
active role of the receiver is when counterarguing
takes place. Counter arguing is the disagreeing with a message that
takes place in people's minds during exposure to the message. Of course, the
opposite—supportive argumentation—can also occur (Smith, 1982).
Example of the new approach to persuasion
theory is the information integration theory developed by Norman Anderson
(1971, 1981), among others. This approach postulates that attitude change is a
result of the way individuals combine or integrate all the information
available relevant to a given attitude object. As a person receives a new piece
of information judged to be relevant to the attitude object, the person assigns
a weight and a value to that information. The weight
is the person's subjective belief in the truth of the information. The value is
defined as the person's affective evaluation of the piece of information, and
it may range from extremely positive to extremely negative. The weight rating
and the value rating can be multiplied to obtain the importance rating for the piece of information.
It is not clear yet how the various
pieces of information (or ratings) are combined to arrive at a final attitude.
Some scholars have argued for a model in which the ratings are added, while
others have argued for a model in which the ratings are averaged.
Comments
Post a Comment